This article is available in:
HTML
PDF
The skill component of Texas hold'em online poker (THOP) adds a unique element to the assessment of risk for problem gambling
(PG). The current study examined whether PG among a high earning subgroup of THOP players was analogous to workaholism. Participants
were self-selected online poker players (N = 31), and results revealed that participants played an average of 30.5 hours per week, and had an average annual online
poker earning of CAD 29 995. Furthermore, 32% of the sample gambled problematically according to the PGSI. In accordance with
previous findings, PG was uniquely predicted by time played and stress. However, PG in this subset of THOP players was associated
with an external locus of control. Contrary to expectations, the personality variable of neuroticism was unrelated to PG and
workaholism. Furthermore, workaholism was unrelated to any variables in the model, and no significant relationship emerged
between workaholism and PG.
Les habiletés requises pour s'adonner au jeu en ligne Texas Hold'em Online Poker (THOP) ajoutent un élément inédit à l'évaluation
du risque associé aux problèmes de jeu. Nous avons cherché à déterminer s'il était possible d'établir une analogie entre le
jeu problématique chez un groupe de joueurs de THOP qui remportent gros et l'ergomanie. L'échantillon (n = 31) était composé
de participants volontaires. Les résultats montrent que ceux-ci consacrent en moyenne 30,5 heures par semaine au jeu en ligne
et que leurs gains annuels moyens s'élèvent à CAD 29 995. Par ailleurs, 32 % d'entre eux se classaient dans la catégorie des
joueurs compulsifs suivant l'IGJC. D'après des résultats antécédents, le temps passé à jouer et le degré de stress seraient
les seuls facteurs de prédiction du jeu problématique. Chez le groupe de joueurs de THOP, toutefois, les problèmes de jeu
sont associés à un locus de contrôle externe. Contrairement à nos attentes, la variable du névrosisme n'était pas associée
aux problèmes de jeu et à l'ergomanie. De plus, aucun lien n'a été relevé entre l'ergomanie et les autres variables du modèle;
aucun lien significatif n'est ressorti entre l'ergomanie et le jeu problématique.
The worldwide play of online Texas Hold'em Poker (THOP) in the last decade raises concerns about the possibility of increased
problem gambling rates in online poker players. However, the skill component of THOP may add a unique element to the assessment
of risk of problem gambling in THOP players, in contrast to video lottery terminal players and players of other games solely
based on chance (Bjerg, 2010; Hopley & Nicki, 2010). Hopley and Nicki (2010) reasoned that major differences exist between THOP players and other types of gamblers, and suggested that the Problem Gambling
Severity Index (PGSI) (Ferris & Wynne, 2001) and other measures of problem gambling may not validly predict problem gambling in this demographic. In a direct comparison
of professional and pathological gamblers, the latter score higher on problem gambling screeners, impulsivity, mental health
symptomatology and Axis I diagnoses, and lower in quality of life, self-esteem, perceived social support, gambling self-efficacy,
and past-90-day gambling frequency (Weinstock, Massura, & Petry, 2013). Consequently, the measurement of problem gambling among THOP players who either earn a living or supplement their income
with winnings might better be reconceptualised to consider this fact: The motivational basis for excessive play by THOP players
pertains more to work than to entertainment.
Hopley and Nicki (2010) investigated the association between variables regularly linked to problem gambling in general (i.e., impulsivity, boredom
proneness, negative emotions, and dissociation) and problem gambling in online poker players. The authors found that hours
played per week, dissociation, boredom proneness, impulsivity, and negative emotionality were all predictive of problem gambling
among online poker players, as defined by the PGSI. Furthermore, the amount of hours spent playing THOP was significantly
and positively correlated with PGSI scores. Noteworthy was the finding that on average, participants reported having won rather
than lost money. Specifically, 34% of the sample made a living or supplemented their income with their online poker earnings.
With respect to other kinds of gambling activity, such as blackjack, this outcome would be a rare one.
The findings of Hopley and Nicki (2010) were replicated and extended by Hopley, Dempsey, and Nicki (2012), who asserted that, because of the skill component of THOP, poker players differed from other types of gamblers. Furthermore,
they proposed that certain questions of the current PGSI may not be applicable to this skilled and high-earning subset of
the THOP population. For example, the question “thinking about the past 12 months, how often have you gone back another day
to try to win back the money you lost?” may not be relevant to poker players who earn a living or supplement their income
with their online poker winnings, since the nature of their work requires them to continue playing to earn a living. This
viewpoint is supported by the Orford, Wardle, Griffiths, Sproston, and Erens' study (2010), which revealed that the presence of the question pertaining to chasing the gambler's losses in both the PGSI and the Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) lowered the internal consistency of these measures. Therefore, the PGSI may
incorrectly categorize this specific high-earning population as problem gamblers because of the nature of certain questions.
In the current study, we adopted a different approach to this issue, and investigated whether problem gambling among this
specific population was analogous to workaholism. That is, poker players who consider online poker playing as a job that provides
their primary or secondary source of income may have a different relationship to playing poker than people who play solely
for enjoyment. Accordingly, THOP players who devote significant hours to online play may be better characterized as being
overly absorbed in work. This argument is supported by the findings of Weinstock and colleagues (2013) who noted that professional gamblers had increased frequency of gambling in the previous 90 days compared to problem gamblers.
For those players who use THOP as employment, playing for greater amount of hours might well be associated with increased
earnings, similar to working increased hours. At the same time, because the high-earning population of THOP players spend
many hours playing THOP, they would presumably be more prone to the development of problem gambling. Thus, the adoption of
a complementary approach to the assessment of problem gambling in this special population is warranted, an approach that involves
a consideration of how the construct of workaholism applies to THOP players.
Workaholism refers to the widely acknowledged phenomenon occurring when an individual becomes overly involved in work life,
has difficulty disengaging from work, and continually thinks about work. As a result, other significant life aspects tend
to suffer, such as relationships (Griffiths, 2005). However, workaholism is not associated with financial strain, much in the same way excessive online poker play may be linked
to increased financial reward. This definition also relates to a number of diagnostic items of pathological gambling of the
DSM-IV criteria: (A1) the individual demonstrates a preoccupation with gambling; (A3) the individual has repeated unsuccessful
efforts to control, cut back, or stop gambling; (A7) the individual lies to family members, therapist or other persons to
conceal the extent of involvement with gambling; and, finally, (A9) the individual has jeopardized or lost a significant relationship,
job, educational, or career opportunity because of gambling (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). For a detailed comparison between problem gambling and workaholism, see Table 1.
Workaholism is typically conceptualized from a trait theory perspective. This viewpoint maintains that workaholic behaviour
develops and progresses from stable personality traits, as well as internal and external stressors, and is maintained by reinforcement
(Burke, Matthiesen, & Pallesen, 2006; Griffiths, 2005). In addition, the personality variable of neuroticism may predispose individuals to workaholic behaviour by increasing the
likelihood of experiencing negative emotional states, such as anxiety, anger, guilt and depressed mood (Burke et al., 2006; Myrseth, Pallesen, Molde, Johnsen, & Lorvik, 2009). Consistent with this viewpoint, stress was found by Spence and Robbins (1992) to be linked to workaholism, and also was found to be uniquely predictive of problem gambling by Hopley and colleagues (2012). The latter finding was consistent with earlier reports, by Coman, Burrows, and Evans (1997), of an association between negative mood states such as depression, anxiety, and stress and the development and maintenance
of problem gambling.
Spence and Robbins (1992) proposed the existence of three dimensions of workaholism: Work Involvement (WI), Enjoyment (E), and Drive (D) (McMillan, Brady, O'Driscoll, & Marsh, 2002). The WI dimension consists of the level of psychological involvement with work. As individuals high in workaholism can enjoy
and be satisfied with their work, the E dimension pertains to the level of pleasure with work. Lastly, the D dimension refers
to the degree of intrinsic personal ambition—that is, an internal demand rather than an external pressure to work and succeed.
Burke and colleagues (2006) conducted a study examining the association between the Big Five personality factors (neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness,
agreeableness, openness) and the three components of workaholism. They found a significant relationship between neuroticism,
extraversion, and conscientiousness, and the three workaholism components. In addition, Spence and Robbins (1992) found a moderate positive correlation between hours worked and overall workaholism levels, a finding comparable to the findings
of Hopley and colleagues (2012) and Hopley and Nicki (2010), and which indicated that problem gambling was uniquely predicted by time played. Therefore, spending more time playing THOP
may increase both the risk of workaholism and problem gambling in this population. However, the converse remains possible,
as high levels of workaholism may also engender an increased risk of playing excessive amounts of online poker. Longitudinal
research is lacking, and therefore, directionality has yet to be determined.
Finally, locus of control (LOC) refers to an individual's perception about the underlying causes leading to an outcome (Duttweiler, 1984; Rotter, 1954). High internal LOC involves the perception of personal control over the outcome, whereas high external LOC involves the
perception that the outcome is dependent on external causes, such as fate, luck, or powerful others (Duttweiler, 1984; Rotter, 1954). Hopley and colleagues (2012) found that internal LOC was a unique predictor of problem gambling among THOP players: people classified as problem gamblers
had greater internal LOC. This finding was supported by Lester's (1980) report that individuals with a high external LOC tend to play games of luck more frequently rather than games of skill, such
as poker. With regard to THOP, a high internal LOC should foster the perception of greater control over the outcome of the
game in skilled players.
This study aimed to examine whether problem gambling among people who earn a living or supplement their income with their
online poker winnings was analogous to workaholism. Therefore, we investigated the relationship between the current PGSI (Ferris & Wynne, 2001) and Spence and Robbins' (1992) Work-BAT (Workaholism Battery). It was hypothesized that there would be a positive correlation between these two criterion
variables, with high scores on the PGSI relating to high scores on the Work-BAT. Furthermore, an exploration of the relationship
between stress, neuroticism, and locus of control, with respect to the PGSI and the Work-BAT, was undertaken using a canonical
correlation analysis in one model. We hypothesized that high scores among all of the three variables—stress, neuroticism,
and LOC—would be related to high scores in both the PGSI and the Work-BAT.
Participants were self-selected THOP players with average annual online poker earnings of at least $10,000 who were recruited
from poker forums and advertising on social networking sites (all dollar figures in this article are presented as Canadian
dollars). A total of 102 participants (80 males, 5 females, 17 undisclosed) started the survey; after screening, 31 male participants
completed the entire survey and provided usable data. Participants were screened out if they were multivariate outliers, the
questionnaires were unscorable, or they dropped out. The mean age of the screened sample was 28.77 (19–53, SD = 8.81). Participants primarily identified as being Caucasian (64.5%) or other (25.8%), and fewer participants identified
as African-American (3.2%), Latino (3.2%), and bi- or multi-racial (3.2%). Approximately 39% of the sample supported themselves
solely with THOP and 19% reported supplementing their incomes with their earnings. The highest 40% of the screened sample
earned between $25,000–120,000; the middle 20% earned between $18,000–25,000; and the lowest 40% earned under $18,000 in net
yearly income.
The online survey was hosted by SurveyMonkey (http://www.surveymonkey.com), a third party surveyor, and one which employs multiple layers of security, layers that maintain the privacy and security
of data. The survey consisted of a general demographics questionnaire that included questions pertaining to age, sex, race
and education, followed by a gambling demographics questionnaire that contained questions relating to the amount of hours
spent playing poker in a typical week, multi-tabling (amount of games played at once), and yearly net profit. Specifically,
participants were asked “on average, how much real money do you wager on Texas Hold'em poker in one day?” and “since starting
to play Texas Hold'em poker, approximately what is your yearly net profit or net loss?” The criterion used to capture the
specific high earning population was a cut-off value of players who earned at least $10,000 annually. This cut-off was chosen
because Hopley & Nicki (2010) and Hopley and colleagues (2012) had discovered that $10,000 represented earnings at the 60th and 70th percentiles respectively. Furthermore, the aforementioned studies noted that THOP players report playing as a primary source
or supplement to their income. A cut-off of $10,000 is roughly equivalent to yearly part-time work at minimum wage, and when
paired with part-time income would place the player above the Canadian after-tax low income cut-off rate (i.e., poverty line),
according to Statistics Canada, for a one person family.
The negative emotional symptom of stress was assessed using the stress subscale of the DASS (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). Overall, the DASS possesses strong discriminant and convergent validity, distinguishing between the three negative emotional
syndromes, which supports the use of this subscale (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). Research reveals that the DASS is reliable and valid for use in an online format (Zlomke, 2009). The stress subscale of the DASS has been found to have very good internal consistency (α = .94) (Zlomke, 2009). A Cronbach's Alpha value of .95 was found in the current study. Previous research has identified that the test-retest reliability
of the online format across a six week period was adequate (r = .57–.67) (Zlomke, 2009). Furthermore, the means and standard deviations for the online administered DASS were not significantly different from the
results of the paper-and-pencil form (Zlomke, 2009). The 14-item stress subscale of the DASS has participants rate themselves on a 4-point scale from “did not apply to me at
all” to “applied to me very much, or most of the time.” The scores on the stress subscale range from 0 to 42, with high scores
reflecting more stress and low scores indicating less stress.
LOC was assessed using Duttweiler's (1984) ICI. The ICI has good reliability and convergent validity (Duttweiler, 1984; Goodman & Waters, 1987). It also enjoys good internal reliability (α = .84) (Duttweiler, 1984). In this study, a Cronbach's Alpha value of .83 was found, a value that is a good level of internal consistency. The 28-item
ICI has participants rate themselves on a 5-point scale from “rarely” to “usual,” where half of the items are reversed scored.
The scores on the ICI range from 28 to 140, with high scores indicating a higher internal LOC and low scores reflecting an
external LOC.
Neuroticism was assessed using the neuroticism subscale of Costa and McCrae's (1992) NEO-PI-R. The domains of the NEO-PI-R have shown good internal consistency (α = .88–.92) (Gaughan, Miller, Pryor, & Lynam, 2009). Furthermore, the NEO-PI-R has demonstrated both convergent and discriminant validity across multiple empirical examinations
(Costa & McCrae, 1992; Morey et al., 2002). However, in this study, a low Cronbach's Alpha value (α = .41) was found for the neuroticism subscale; therefore, results pertaining to neuroticism herein must be interpreted
with caution. The 20-item neuroticism subscale has participants rate themselves on a 5-point scale from “very inaccurate”
to “very accurate.” Scores on the neuroticism subscale range from 20 to 100, with high scores indicating a higher tendency
towards emotional instability.
Workaholism was assessed using Spence and Robbins' (1992) Work-BAT. Spence and Robbins (1992) found that the Work-BAT contained face validity, adequate to good internal consistency (α = .67–.86), and adequate convergent validity. The WI component has been found to have good internal consistency (α = .80) (McMillan et al., 2002). The E domain also has shown evidence of having high internal consistency (α = .85) (McMillan et al., 2002). The D subscale has yielded acceptable internal consistency (α = .74; Spence & Robbins, 1992). In this study, a Cronbach's Alpha value of α = .85 was found for the Work-BAT. The 25-item Work-BAT has participants
rate themselves on a 5-point scale from “very untrue of me” to “very true of me.” Scores on the Work-BAT range from 25 to
125. High scores indicate a higher tendency to display workaholic behaviours.
Finally, gambling severity was assessed using the PGSI subsection of the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). The PGSI has shown evidence of high internal consistency (α = .90), internal reliability, unidimensionality, and good item-response characteristics (Abbott & Volberg, 2006; Hopley et al., 2012). A Cronbach's Alpha value of α = .88 was found in this study. The PGSI and DSM-IV measures of problem gambling are strongly correlated and distinguishes
problem gamblers from non-problem gamblers to a reasonable extent (r = .65) (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). The 9-item PGSI has participants rate themselves on a 4-point scale from “never” to “almost always.” A total score of zero
indicates a non-problem gambler, total scores of one and two show a low risk for problem gambling, total scores of three to
seven points towards moderate risk, and total scores eight and above reflect problem gambling (Ferris & Wynne, 2001).
Individuals who viewed the online advertisement were offered a chance to win one of four $50 prizes. A random number generator
was then used to select four participant ID numbers, who were then contacted by electronic mail and their inducements were
transferred via Paypal. Once individuals clicked on the link, they were brought to the consent form outlining the nature of
the study, the confidential and anonymous nature of their responses, and their right to withdraw. Participants then completed
a demographics questionnaire, followed by a gambling demographics questionnaire. Participants then proceeded to complete the
questionnaires measuring the variables of interest in the study; the order of the questionnaires was randomized. In total,
participants answered 103 questions, a process which took approximately 30–45 minutes to complete.
Prior to the analysis, the data were conditioned to meet the assumptions of regression. The assumption of normality was met,
as skewness and kurtosis were absent. Furthermore, missing value analysis indicated that the overall pattern of missing data
corresponded to the pattern: missing completely at random (MCAR), as Little's MCAR Test was non-significant (χ2 = 0.00; df = 1136, p > .05). Therefore, listwise deletion was used to eliminate participants with incomplete data, as missing values were not
dependent on any measured variables. One multivariate outlier was removed from the original sample, and no univariate outliers
were found with regard to questionnaire scores.
Participants reported a variety of hours played per week with minimum amount of 5 hours and a maximum amount of 70 hours (M = 30.55; SD = 15.90). The question relating to multi-tabling revealed that participants played from one to 17 games at one time (M = 5.00; SD = 3.75). Average annual online poker earning ranged from a minimum profit of $10,000 to a maximum profit of $120,000 with
a median value of $22,000 (M = 29,994.85; SD = 23,936.84). PGSI scores varied from 0 to 21 (M = 6.10; SD = 5.34). Based on PGSI scores, 9.68% of the participants were non-problem gamblers; 22.58% were low-risk gamblers; 35.48%
were moderate-risk gamblers, and 32.26% of the sample consisted of problem gamblers. Finally, total Work-BAT scores varied
from a minimum score of 55 to a maximum score of 112 (M = 82.29; SD = 15.01). While no cut-off scores were proposed to distinguish workaholics from non-workaholics, participants in this study
reported moderate-high elevations in workaholism levels.
Pearson correlations were calculated for all pairs of variables. Moderate negative correlations were found between neuroticism
and time spent playing online poker (r = −.40, p < .05), LOC and stress (r = −.43, p < .05), and PGSI scores and LOC (r = −.56, p < .05). PGSI scores were moderately positively correlated with stress (r = .56, p < .01), and time spent playing THOP (r = .42, p < .05). Workaholism was not significantly correlated with time spent playing online poker. Furthermore, workaholism and problem
gambling were not significantly correlated. Problem gambling was not significantly correlated with the Drive subscale of the
Work-BAT.
To examine the predictors of problematic gambling and their relationship to both problem gambling and workaholism, a canonical
correlation analysis was conducted with two canonical variable sets. Canonical variate X consisted of PGSI total score and
Work-BAT total score, whereas Canonical variate Y was made up of weekly hours played, DASS stress scores, ICI total score,
and Neuroticism total score. This analysis determined which combination of predictors best related to problem gambling and
workaholism through a linear combination of weighted measures (see Figure 1).
The Wilks multivariate test was significant (Wilks λ = .37, F(8,50) = 3.92, p < .01) with an overall effect size of R2 = .62. Of two possible canonical correlations, only one was significant, rc = .74 (54% overlapping variance). The relationship between dependent variables and Canonical variate X was strong for problem
gambling scores (r = .98) and weak for workaholism scores (r = −.11). The relationship between predictors and Canonical variate Y was moderate for hours played (r = .62) and neuroticism (r = −.34) strong for stress (r = .78) and locus of control scores (r = −.71). The dependent variables account for 26.44% of the variance in Canonical variate X, whereas the predictors accounted
for 21.75% of the variance in Canonical variate Y. The canonical function (i.e., the pair of canonical variates) consisted
of a positive relationship between time played and stress, and a negative relationship between LOC and problem gambling scores,
indicating that an external LOC was predictive of problem gambling in this subsample of THOP players. Both workaholism and
neuroticism made only minimal contributions to the model.
In this study, we investigated an alternative viewpoint of playing THOP involving workaholism in addition to problem gambling
among THOP players who earned a living or supplemented their income with their online poker winnings. This was a novel step
towards a greater understanding of problem gambling in this population. The relationship between the PGSI and the Work-BAT
was examined. It was hypothesized that there would be a positive correlation between these two variables, with high scores
on the PGSI relating to high scores on the Work-BAT. The respective relationships of three variables—stress, neuroticism,
and LOC to the PGSI and the Work-BAT—were assessed. In accordance with the findings of Hopley and colleagues (2012), it was expected that stress and internal locus of control would be significant predictors of problem gambling. Furthermore,
we expected that scores among all of the three variables (locus of control, stress, and neuroticism) would significantly overlap
with the canonical variate pertaining to the combined PGSI and Work-BAT.
The results of the current study partially replicated the findings of Hopley and colleagues (2012), in that we found that hours spent playing online poker, stress, and LOC were significant predictors of problem gambling.
However, the personality variable of neuroticism was not found to significantly predict problem gambling as hypothesized.
Furthermore, none of the variables were significant predictors of workaholism, contrary to expectations; nor did workaholism
load onto the canonical variate with problem gambling. While LOC was related to problem gambling, a strong negative correlation
was found. Therefore, among high earning online THOP players, an external LOC was predictive of problem gambling, contrary
to the findings of Hopley and colleagues (2012).
In the current study, a large number of the participants were identified as problem gamblers (32.25% of the sample). This
is approximately twice the amount that was found in both Hopley and Nicki (2010) and Hopley and colleagues (2012) studies. In this study, because our target population was high-earning THOP players, this finding reinforces our concerns
about the validity of the PGSI as an assessment of problem gambling among this demographic. Alternatively, this may indicate
that high earning THOP players are at increased risk for the development of problem gambling, when compared with casual players.
At the same time, the findings of this study do not support the use of the Work-BAT as a measure of problem playing of THOP
gamblers.
Time spent playing online poker was found to be a significant predictor of problem gambling in accord with Hopley and colleagues' (2012) findings; however, time was not related to workaholism in the canonical model. The former result supports previous research,
a finding which suggests that the ease of access to online games may increase the opportunity to play. This increase may lead
to greater risk for problem gambling (Hopley et al., 2012). Because Spence and Robbins (1992) found a moderate positive correlation between hours worked and Work-BAT scale scores, we expected that time spent playing
online poker would be a significant predictor of workaholism. However, the results did not support this hypothesis, and indicate
instead that an increased amount of hours playing THOP leads to problem gambling but not workaholism.
When the effect of stress was analyzed in relation to problem gambling and workaholism, it was found to be strongly related
only to problem gambling, which replicated Hopley and colleagues' (2012) findings. This correspondence is consistent with research by Coman and colleagues (1997), who found that individuals use gambling as a way to reduce stress that is caused by other unpleasant emotional states, and
the association between these variables hypothesized in this study. On the other hand, Spence and Robbins (1992) found a link between stress and workaholism, one which was consistent with our hypothesis, but not supported by our findings.
Therefore, the relationship between stress and problem gambling in THOP gamblers may be fundamentally different from the relationship
between stress and workaholism in THOP gamblers.
LOC was found to be a significant predictor of problem gambling, but in the direction opposite to our expectations, and contrary
to the findings of Hopley and colleagues (2012). That is, an external LOC was related to higher scores on the PGSI. Previous research has linked LOC to problem gambling,
in that problem gamblers develop illusory perceptions of control over the outcome, and, as a result, develop an internal LOC
(Carroll & Huxley, 1994). However, this explanation would not be applicable to THOP players, who differ from other types of gamblers because of the
presence of a skill component in THOP, one where perceptions of control may indeed reflect reality. Therefore, it seems reasonable
to expect that feelings of control would occur in situations where skillful players are controlling the games and consistently
winning, and that losses would be attributed by them as chance-outcome events. Furthermore, among highly-skilled players,
loss of this perception of control may reflect or precipitate movement towards problem gambling. Conversely, less skilled
players may increase their vulnerability to problem gambling by possessing a strong internal locus of control (Hopley et al., 2012). A relationship between LOC and workaholism was not found. This would suggest that LOC is not a variable linked to workaholism,
and that variations in LOC do not have an effect on workaholic behaviour, or vice versa.
The personality variable of neuroticism did not contribute to the canonical variates, and was unrelated to problem gambling
and workaholism, contrary to hypotheses. Our hypothesis was partly based on Burke, and colleagues' (2006) findings that there was a significant relationship between neuroticism, extraversion, and conscientiousness, and the three
workaholism components. Neuroticism, in particular, was positively related to feeling driven to work (Burke et al., 2006). However, in testing for this effect, we did not find any significant results. In regard to problem gambling, Myrseth and colleagues (2009) and Bagby and colleagues (2007) found the neuroticism was significantly related to problem gambling. In the current study, however, a moderate negative correlation
was found between neuroticism and time spent playing online poker; therefore, neuroticism increased while time spent playing
online poker decreased or vice versa. Unfortunately, conclusions about the relationship between neuroticism and both problem
gambling and workaholism cannot be drawn because of the weak psychometric properties of the NEO-PI-R used in this study; therefore,
replication is necessary.
The current study possessed several limitations that must be acknowledged. First, there was a large discrepancy between our
target sample size of 100 participants and our obtained sample size of 31 participants, a discrepancy which reduced the statistical
power of the study. Furthermore, the self-report nature of the questionnaires limits the validity of the responses as response
biases (e.g., social desirability, deception) may have influenced the nature of the responses. Moreover, although we had a
large number of participants who initially began the survey, relatively few participants completed it; therefore, the sample
may be biased and, consequently, the generalization of the findings is correspondingly reduced. This possibility may in turn
indicate a limitation of our measures, and suggests that briefer measures should be adopted for future research. In addition,
the entirety of participants in the study were male; therefore, the findings cannot be generalized to female THOP players.
As noted above, the very low internal consistency found for the NEO-PI-R prevented conclusions to be drawn surrounding that
measure, and this level of consistency may explain why we were unable to identify any significant relationships with the theoretically
relevant variable or neuroticism. Lastly, as noted earlier, certain questions on the PGSI may not be appropriate, and may
not predict with validity problem gambling in those THOP players who earn a living or supplement their income with their online
poker winnings.
Although there are concerns with the validity of the PGSI, workaholism was found to be unrelated to problem gambling and,
therefore, not a meaningful construct for triangulating problem gambling. The current study was the first to propose the possible
connection between problem gambling among THOP players who earn a living or supplement their income with their winnings, and
workaholism. The absence of any significant findings with regard to workaholism could be attributed to the small sample size,
or that the two variables, problem gambling and workaholism, measure very different constructs, ones which may not in fact
complement each other. In addition, to target the higher earning population of THOP players, we used a cut-off rate of $10,000
average annual online poker earnings. However, the use of this cut-off rate may have been too arbitrary, and may not have
been the optimal way of recruiting participants who consider THOP as a primary source of income. A possible alternative approach
to overcoming this issue would have been to use a screening test to capture a more detailed representation of the participants'
work situation relating to poker, and to gain more information about their poker earnings. Furthermore, it may be beneficial
to compare scores on the Work-BAT between THOP players and people in different occupations who work for a similar amount of
hours and have approximately the same income. Such a project would allow us to identify if workaholism could, in fact, be
applicable to THOP as a work occupation, and also control for certain extraneous variables.
As suggested by Hopley and colleagues (2012), a longitudinal study further investigating the development and maintenance of stress would help overcome the issue of causality.
Furthermore, as mentioned above, high internal LOC feelings may be appropriate in this successful sample, and future investigations
to explain this discrepancy between the current study and those of Hopley and colleagues (2012) are warranted. Future research should investigate this relationship among losing players, as well as the relationship between
LOC and skill in THOP players. The pursuit of these questions would be consistent with the recommendation by Bjerg (2010) that distinctions should be made among losing, break-even, and winning players to understand more fully problem gambling
in poker players. With respect to the personality variable of neuroticism, it would be beneficial to examine other personality
traits and dimensions suggested to be linked to problem gambling, such as openness and conscientiousness, with questionnaires
that have stronger psychometric properties. Finally, time played has consistently emerged as a behavioural predictor of online
problem gambling; therefore, gambling frequency and time played could be used to identify problematic gambling patterns and
separate these from professional play, furthering the findings of Weinstock and colleagues (2013). Behavioural data centering on amount wagered or net income (i.e., regular versus high stakes online play), initially examined
in this study, should be replicated with regard to differentiating between problem gamblers and normative play.
In brief, this study partially replicated the findings of Hopley and colleagues (2012) because it found that stress and time spent playing online poker were significant predictors of problem gambling; however,
an external LOC was also strongly related to problem gambling. Furthermore, this study attempted to adopt a unique approach
in the assessment of problem gambling among high earning THOP players, from a workaholism perspective. Consequently, we found
that the two constructs were not related; therefore, workaholism is likely not an appropriate alternative or complementary
measure of problem gambling. The results of this study, however, may further initiate the development of a valid measure of
problem gambling among THOP players who earn a living or supplement their income with their online poker winnings.
Canonical model representing the optimal linear combination of variables and their degree of overlap Copyright © 2020 | Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
Journal Information
Journal ID (publisher-id): jgi
ISSN: 1910-7595
Publisher: Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
Article Information
The Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
Received Day: 12 Month: December Year: 2012
Accepted Day: 10 Month: September Year: 2013
Publication date: October 2014
First Page: 1 Last Page: 17
Publisher Id: jgi.2014.29.5
DOI: 10.4309/jgi.2014.29.5
Making a living online: Problem gambling and workaholism in high earning online Texas hold'em poker players
1Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
2University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, New Brunswick, Canada nick@unb.ca
This article was peer-reviewed. All URLs were available at the time of submission.
For correspondence: Richard Nicki, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of New Brunswick, 38 Dineen Drive, Room 126,
Keirstead Hall, Fredericton, New Brunswick, E3B 5A3. Tel: (506) 452-6104. Fax: (506) 447-3063. E-mail: nick@unb.ca
Competing interests: None declared (all authors).
Ethics approval: The Research Ethics Board for Human Research of the University of New Brunswick approved on December 9, 2010
“Making a living online: Problem gambling and workaholism in high earning online Texas Hold‘em poker players” (10-39-1).
Funding: This research was supported by the Department of Health of the New Brunswick provincial government.
Contributors: Authors are listed in order of contribution. The idea for the project was derived as an extension of the previous
work of AH and RN. Both AH and JW conducted literature searches, and JW completed an initial draft of the paper for an honour's
thesis. JW completed preliminary analyses while AH performed more advanced statistical analyses and interpretations. Subsequently,
AH and RN jointly edited and revised the initial thesis into the current manuscript.
Anthony Hopley is a PhD candidate at the University of New Brunswick and is completing his pre-doctoral internship at the
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health in Toronto, Ontario. Both branches of his research interests center on addiction. First,
he is completing his dissertation, which is focused on the development of a comprehensive typology of substance misusing offenders.
Second, he has investigated the predictors and correlates of excessive online Texas Hold'em Poker.
Jennifer Wagner, BA, completed her honour's thesis under the supervision of her co-authors.
Dr. Richard Nicki is a Professor Emeritus in the Department of Psychology at the University of New Brunswick in Fredericton.
He is a licensed psychologist, and a specialist in the areas of motivation and cognitive behaviour therapy. For many years,
his laboratory research group has investigated factors relating to the causes and prevention of problem gambling in video
lottery terminal and online Texas Hold'em Poker players.
Abstract
Résumé
References
Abbott, M. W..
, &
Volberg, R. A. . ( 2006). The measurement of adult problem and pathological gambling. International Gambling Studies, 6(2), 175–200.
doi: 10.1081/14459790600928678
American Psychiatric Association ( 2000). Diagnostic and Statistical manual of mental disorders(4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC:Author.
doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349
Bagby, R. M..
,
Vachon, D. D..
,
Bulmash, E. L..
,
Toneatto, T..
,
Quilty, L. C..
, &
Costa, P. T. . ( 2007). Pathological gambling and the fivefactor model of personality.. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 873–880.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.011
Bjerg, O. . ( 2010). Problem gambling in poker Money, rationality and control in a skillbased social game.. International Gambling Studies, 10(3), 239–254.
doi:10/1080/14459795.2010.520330
Burke, R. J..
,
Matthiesen, S. B..
, &
Pallesen, S. . ( 2006). Personality correlates of workaholism. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(6), 1223–1233.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.017
Carroll, D..
, &
Huxley, J. A. A. . ( 1994). Cognitive, dispositional, and psychophysiological correlates of dependent slot machine gambling in young people. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(12), 1070–1083.
doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb02374.x
Coman, G. J..
,
Burrows, G. D..
, &
Evans, B. J. . ( 1997). Stress and anxiety as factors in the onset of problem gambling Implications for treatment.. Stress Medicine, 13(4), 235–244.
doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1700(199710)13:4<235::AID-SMI748>3.0.CO;2-4
Costa, P. T..
, &
McCrae, R. R. . ( 1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO- PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Duttweiler, P. C. . ( 1984). The internal control index A newly developed measure of locus of control.. Education and Psychological Measurement, 44(2), 209–221.
doi:10.1177/0013164484442004
Ferris, J..
, &
Wynne, H. . ( 2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final Report. Report to the Canadian Inter-Provincial Advisory Committee. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Consortium for Gambling Research.
Gaughan, E. T..
,
Miller, J. D..
,
Pryor, L. R..
, &
Lynam, D. R. . ( 2009). Comparing two alternative measures of general personality in the assessment of psychopathy A test of the NEOPIR and the MPQ.. Journal of Personality, 77(4), 965–996.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00571.x
Goodman, S. H..
, &
Waters, L. K. . ( 1987). Convergent validity of five locus of control scales. Education and Psychological Measurements, 47(3), 743–747.
doi: 10.1177/001316448704700326
Griffiths, M. . ( 2005). Workaholism is still a useful construct. Addiction Research and Theory, 13(2), 97–100.
doi: 10.1080/16066350500057290
Hopley, A. A. B..
,
Dempsey, K..
, &
Nicki, R. . ( 2012). Texas Holdem Online Poker A further examination.. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10(4), 563–572.
doi: 10.1007/s11469-011-9353-2
Hopley, A. A. B..
, &
Nicki, R. M. . ( 2010). Predictive factors of excessive online poker playing. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(4), 379–385.
doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0223
Lester, D. . ( 1980). Choice of gambling activity and belief in locus of control. Psychological Reports, 47(1), 22.
doi: 10.2466/pr0.1980.47.1.22
Lovibond, P. F..
, &
Lovibond, S. H. . ( 1995). The structure of negative emotional states Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales DASS with the Beck Depression
and Anxiety Inventories.. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.
doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
McMillan, L. W. H..
,
Brady, E. C..
,
O'Driscoll, M. P..
, &
Marsh, N. V. . ( 2002). A multifaceted validation study of Spence and Robbins 1992 Workaholism Battery.. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 75(3), 357–368.
doi: 10.1348/096317902320369758
Morey, L. C..
,
Quigley, B. D..
,
Sanislow, C. A..
,
Skodol, A. E..
,
McGlashan, T. H..
,
Shea, M. T..
, …
Gunderson, J. G. . ( 2002). Substance or style An investigation of the NEOPIR validity scales.. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79(3), 583–599.
doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7903_11
Myrseth, H..
,
Pallesen, S..
,
Molde, H..
,
Johnsen, B. H..
, &
Lorvik, I. M. . ( 2009). Personality factors as predictors of pathological gambling. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 933–937.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.018
Orford, J..
,
Wardle, H..
,
Griffiths, M..
,
Sproston, K..
, &
Erens, B. . ( 2010). PGSI and DSMIV in the 2007 British Gambling Prevalence Survey Reliability, item response, factor structure and interscale
agreement.. International Gambling Studies, 10(1), 31–44.
doi: 10.1080/14459790903567132
Rotter, J. B. . ( 1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Spence, J. T..
, &
Robbins, A. S. . ( 1992). Workaholism Definition, measurement, and preliminary results.. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58(1), 160–178.
doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5801_15
Weinstock, J..
,
Massura, C. E..
, &
Petry, N. M. . ( 2013). Professional and pathological gamblers Similarities and differences.. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(2), 205–16.
doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9308-y
Zlomke, K. R. . ( 2009). Psychometric properties of internet administered versions of Penn State Worry Questionnaire PSWQ and Depression, Anxiety,
and Stress Scale DASS.. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(4), 841–843.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.06.003
Figures
Figure 1
Tables
Article Categories:
Related Article(s):
Editor-in-chief: Nigel E. Turner, Ph.D.
Managing Editor: Vivien Rekkas, Ph.D. (contact)
Introduction
Method
Participants
Materials
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS)
Internal Control Index (ICI)
Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R)
Workaholism Battery (Work-BAT)
Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI)
Procedure
Results
Data Screening
Gambling and Workaholism Demographics
Pearson Correlations
Linear Combinations
Discussion